In the United States there are approximately 57,000 nonhuman primates kept in laboratories (United States Department of Agriculture, 2000). To safeguard their well-being and their suitability for valid scientific research the following primate-specific characteristics must be taken into consideration in the care and use of these animals.
![]() |
Figure 1. "There is no such thing as a boring animal, rhesus or otherwise; only the conditions under which we keep them can be boring" (Mahoney, 1992, p. 36). Primates have intensive social needs; solitary confinement in a barren cage is a severe punishment for them (Sokol, 1993). |
There are simple ways to provide captive primates with housing and handling conditions that address these five characteristics, thereby promoting both the well-being of the animals and the quality of research conducted with them.
Housing primates in groups is the ideal way to account for their social disposition. Conditions for long-term harmonious group-housing, however, are usually not met in research institutions. Aggression-related injury and social distress can often not be avoided when new groups are formed from previously single-housed individuals. The risk remains inherent even in established groups because of the spatial constraints set by confinement and the artificial instability of social relationships resulting from research and managerial stipulations and from health care considerations. It should be emphasized that these risks can be overcome in research facilities committed to exceptionally high animal husbandry standards (Hartner et al., 2001). Housing the animals in pairs offers a practicable alternative to group-housing. Protocols for the safe formation of compatible pairs of previously single-caged adults of the same sex have been documented for numerous species, including chimpanzees (Fritz and Fritz, 1979), baboons (Jerome and Szostak, 1987), squirrel monkeys (Gwinn, 1996), owl monkeys (Weed and Watson, 1998), marmosets (Jackson, 2001; Majolo et al., 2001) rhesus macaques (Reinhardt, 1989a), stump-tailed macaques (Reinhardt, 1994a), long-tailed macaques (Lynch, 1996), and pig-tailed macaques (Byrum and St. Claire, 1998). While adult animals need to be carefully familiarized before being introduced with each other in the same cage, there is little or no danger involved when new pairs are formed by introducing juvenile individuals with strange single-housed adults or by introducing unfamiliar juveniles with each other (Reinhardt, 1994b).
![]() |
Figure 2. Being housed with a compatible companion –– here two male long-tailed macaques engrossed in social grooming –– is a good compromise for addressing the social needs of primates kept in research facilities. Photo by Richard Lynch, AstraZeneca, Wilmington. |
Choice studies conducted with capuchin monkeys have demonstrated that the animals "value social companionship as they value food: It is a necessity, not a luxury" (Dettmer and Fragaszy, 2000, p. 303). The social companion is the one environmental enrichment option of which the caged primate never gets bored (cf., de Waal, 1992). Partners of compatible pairs spend about the same amount of time interacting with each other as wild animals do (Line et al., 1990; Reinhardt, 1990a; Brent, 1992a) `even' when they have lived together for several years (Ranheim and Reinhardt, 1989). This suggests that pair-housing is a reasonable option for addressing their social needs (Figure 2). The behavioral disorder of self-injurious biting is resistant to occupational therapy attempts (Preilowski et al., 1988; Kinsey et al., 1997; Novak et al., 1998) but can be cured by transferring the afflicted single-caged individuals to a compatible pair-housing setup (Line et al., 1990; Bushong et al., 1992; Reinhardt, 1999). Transferring single-caged individuals to compatible pair-housing not only improves their behavioral health, but it also decreases their susceptibility to physical health problems (Schapiro and Bushong, 1994), particularly to stress-related diseases (Schapiro et al., 2000). The two partners of a compatible pair do not differ from each other in terms of stress-sensitive parameters (Gonzalez et al., 1982; Reinhardt et al., 1991; Eaton et al., 1994). Rather than being a source of distress, the companion serves as a stress buffer in fear-provoking situations such as experiments requiring chair-restraint or tether-restraint (Mason, 1960; Coe et al., 1982; Coelho et al., 1991; Figure 3).
![]() | |
|
The horizontally arranged double cage
can provide a suitable primary enclosure for paired primates. It must be
furnished with two feeders and two elevated resting sites, one in each half of
the cage. A dividing panel with a passage hole close to the back wall of the
cage minimizes the risk of squabbles over access to these essential resources
and makes it possible for the two animals to sometimes get out of each other's
sight. This avoids antagonism while fostering affiliation (Reinhardt and
Reinhardt, 1991). The vertically arranged double cage does not offer
suitable housing conditions because the dominant partner may monopolize the
well-illuminated environment of the upper half thereby forcing the subordinate
partner to spend most of the time in the dim environment of the bottom half (own
unpublished observation; cf., Williams et al., 1988).
Housing primates in pairs does not interfere with husbandry procedures and common management and research protocols (Reinhardt et al., 1989; Schapiro and Bushong, 1994; Shively, 2001; Figure 4). If an animal has to be kept temporarily alone –– e.g., during intensive postoperative care or during a metabolic study –– the caging arrangement must allow the individual animal to maintain visual contact with at least one compatible conspecific (cf., Mahoney, 1992; Lindburg and Coe, 1995) to minimize the stress resulting from social deprivation (cf., Coelho et al., 1991). "We all realize that one is better off with the ups and downs of a social life than without a social life at all. Would it be any different with nonhuman primates?" (de Waal, 1992, p. 86).
![]() | |
|
While social contact and social
interaction with another compatible conspecific is a prerequisite for the
psychological and behavioral health of a primate, a trustful relationship
with the attending personnel is essential for his/her well-being. Giving the
animals names, rather than referring to them as numbered research objects helps
to develop such a relationship (cf., Reese EP 1991; Sokol, 1992; de Waal, 1992).
The compassionate and respectful attitude "conveys to the animal a quiet sense
of assurance on which coping strategies can be developed for dealing with other
stressful aspects of the laboratory" (Wolfle, 1987, p. 1221). The macho-type
person is out of place in the animal room because s/he triggers distress
reactions which will skew scientific findings even before the actual experiment
has started. Typically, the animals will panic when such a person comes into
their room (own unpublished observation; cf., Arluke and Sanders,
1996).
In order to provide not only a physically but also a psychologically comfortable environment, staff time must be set aside for interacting and communicating with the animals in a positive manner every day (cf., Home Office, 1989). "There should be no sharp demarcation between 'good guys' and 'bad guys.' Nonhuman primates are quick to forget, or perhaps forgive, the momentary fear or resentment they feel towards a human being who has just subjected them to an unpleasant experience if a strong bond of trust already exists with that person" (Mahoney, 1992, p. 35). The investigator, veterinarian or technician who pretends to be too busy to deal with the animals other than during experimental or clinical procedures lacks basic professional qualification.
Indoor enclosures of primates should take maximum advantage of the height of the room, allowing the installation of climbing structures and resting surfaces at different levels. Elevated structures not only increase the usable cage volume but also provide environmental enrichment by stimulating the animals to spend a major portion of their time engaged in species-typical arboreal activities. A perch can readily be installed in any cages, `even' in those that are equipped with a movable back-wall (Reinhardt and Pape, 1991; Watson, 1991). The monkey perch should be made of non-metallic, i.e., 'warm' material, and have a sufficiently large diameter so that the animal(s) can sit on it comfortably (Abee, 1985) or, as in the case of squirrel monkeys and lemurs lie flat on it with limbs dangling on either side for balance (own unpublished observation; McGivern, 1993).
The perch must be placed at a height that the animal(s) are able to sit on and under it in an unrestricted, i.e, not crouched position (Reinhardt and Reinhardt, 1999). Monkeys prefer to look out of their enclosure rather than hide in the back of it (van Wagenen, 1950; Reinhardt, 1989b). Elevated fixtures, therefore, have to be placed in such a way that the animal(s) can sit on them in a look-out position in the front of the cage (Figure 5). An adequate number of elevated sites must be provided for socially housed animals to avoid competition over access (Williams et al., 1988). When this condition is met, access to perches helps the animals to avoid social conflicts and foster affiliative relationships (Neveu and Deputte, 1996.)
![]() |
Figure 5. The elevated resting surface should be placed in such a way that the animal can sit on it in a look-out position. |
Nonhuman primates can get extremely frightened when a person dressed in protective garb approaches them. To minimize the stress, there should be sufficiently high structures which allow the animals to show a vertical flight response and retreat to a quasi safe location above human eye level (cf., International Primatological Society, 1993; Kaumanns and Schönmann, 1997; Wakenshaw, 1999). Mobile structures such as swings and ropes are generally less preferred than fixed structures (Williams et al., 1988; Howell et al., 1997), particularly in cages that provide only the bare minimum space for postural adjustment (Kopecky and Reinhardt, 1991).
![]() |
![]() | ||
|
|
Since primates are biologically
programmed to spend the major portion of their time gathering and processing
food, feeding enrichment provides optimal environmental stimulation for
them. The urge to forage is so strong that they will work for food even when
identical food is placed right next to them (Markowitz, 1979; Evans et al.,
1989; Reinhardt, 1994c). Distributing the food on the chain-link or mesh ceiling
of the enclosure rather than in food boxes, troughs or on the floor is perhaps
the simplest way to trigger foraging behavior for standard food and supplemental
food, such as fruits, vegetables and bread (Figure 6). Depending on the size of
the individual food pieces, macaques increase their foraging time more than
100-fold when their daily standard biscuit ration is placed on the mesh ceiling
of their cage rather than in ordinary, freely accessible food boxes (Reinhardt,
1993a). The same effect can be achieved when the food box is remounted a few
centimeters away from its access hole (Reinhardt, 1993b). Dexterity is now
required to maneuver the biscuits through the mesh covering the face of the box
(Figure 7). These two feeding enrichment options use structural elements of the
cage, redesigned in such a way that they serve as primary feeders for the daily
standard food ration. Therefore, no extra time is needed to clean them and bait
them with special treats. The animals only work for food that they actually eat
(Murchison, 1994; Reinhardt and Garza-Schmidt, 2000). This avoids the
accumulation of spoiled foodstuff, a problem which commonly occurs with ordinary
feeders, when the animals hoard food and waste part of it by dropping it on
fecal material.
![]() |
Figure 8. In their natural environment marmosets show 'tree-gouging' and 'exudate-feeding'. A simple gum- feeder can support this behavior under laboratory conditions. Photo by Katie Eckert, University of California, San Francisco. |
Marmosets and tamarins are adapted to probe for embedded food in order
to extract it. Simulating a natural food-source with an artificial device
stocked with gum (McGrew et al., 1986; König et al., 1987; Kelly, 1993) or
baited with raisins mixed with corn cob (Molzen and French, 1989) is cheap and
easy but allows the animals to engage in extensive foraging activities (Figure
8). Chimpanzees are proficient in using twigs to extract termites from logs and
mounds. Simple probefeeder devices filled with sticky foods –– such as
applesauce, mashed bananas, jam –– can encourage this mode of foraging in
captivity (Murphy, 1976; Goodall, 1979; Nash, 1982; Maki et al., 1989; Gilloux
et al., 1992; Perret et al., 1998; Figure 9).
![]()
| ||
Straw, woodchips, woodwool, and argilla
espansa are ideal foraging substrates in the form of deep-litter on the solid
floor of pens (Chamove and Anderson, 1979; Fragaszy and Adams-Curtis, 1991;
Brent, 1992b; Beck, 1995; Brown et al., 1995; Riviello and Misiti, 1995; Baker,
1997) or distributed on trays mounted under the mesh floor of cages (Bryant et
al., 1988; Mahoney, 1992). When mixed with seeds, grain, or other small edible
items such materials promote intensive foraging behavior (Burt and Plat, 1990;
Byrne and Suomi, 1991; Combette and Anderson, 1991; Grief et al., 1992;
Poenisch, 1992; Figure 10). The distracting effect of this feeding enrichment
technique is so strong that it reduces social antagonism in group-housed animals
(Chamove et al., 1982; Boccia, 1989).
![]() | |
|
All captive primates should be fed daily
at least one whole, medium-size fruit or vegetable (Figure 11a,b,c). The time
required by animal care personnel to cut produce into small pieces can be spent
in much more meaningful ways (cf., Smith et al., 1989; Kerridge, 1997). For
example, portioning the daily standard food ration in several rather than a
single feeding, probably does not take more time than chopping the supplemental
produce for the animals, but it increases substantially the amount of time they
can engage in foraging activities (cf., Taylor et al., 1997).
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
Figure 11a,b,c. Primates deserve fresh fruit or vegetables on a daily basis. It would be a waste of time to chop the produce for the animals; they have the time and they enjoy doing it themselves. |
Branch segments of dead deciduous trees
–– red oak disintegrates into flakes that are so small that large quantities
pass sewage drains without clogging them (Reinhardt, 1992a) –– are perfect toys
for primates, stimulating not only processing but also manipulative and play
activities (Figure 12). They constantly change their form and texture due to
wear and dehydration and, therefore, retain their stimulatory value (Reinhardt,
1989b; Eckert et al., 2000). Commercial toys lack the natural, ever-changing
texture of wood; this is probably the reason why the animals quickly lose
interest in them (Crockett et al., 1989; Line et al., 1989; Hamilton, 1991;
Pruetz and Bloomsmith, 1992; Kessel and Brent, 1998), unless several different
toys are offered and substituted regularly with new ones (cf., Paquette and
Prescott, 1988; Weick et al., 1991). Access to a variety of manipulable objects
seems to be particularly beneficial for capuchins and baboons, who exhibit
sustained interest in them and respond with a significant reduction in abnormal
behaviors (Brent and Belik, 1997; Boinski et al., 1999).
![]() | |
|
A major contention is the need for
proper illumination in the caging arrangement of medium- and small-sized
primates. In order to minimize housing expenses, these animals are traditionally
kept in two-tier cages, with one row stacked on top of another. This doubles the
number of primates that can be accommodated in one room, but involves serious
implications for the individual animals. Those relegated to the lower rows are
restricted to a terrestrial lifestyle, unable to withdraw in alarming situations
and retreat to a safe place above the human predators who periodically capture
them and subject them to distressing, or even deadly procedures. Moreover, the
sanitation tray, which runs the length of the room beneath the upper tier of
cages, reduces significantly the amount of light that can penetrate to the
lower-cage tier (Schapiro et al., 2000); "animals in the lower tier are thus
relegated to a permanent state of semi-gloom" (Mahoney, 1992; p. 32). The
cave-like living quarters of bottom-row caged animals is often so dim that
caretakers routinely have to use flashlights to identify and inspect them
(Figure 13). It has been noticed in marmosets that the housing environment of
lower-row caged animals can be so poor that it results in markedly reduced
fertility (Heger et al., 1986). Routinely rotating animals between bottom and
top tiers (National Research Council, 1998) offers no solution to this problem.
It merely "rotates" the problem by alleviating the situation for lower-row
subjects, while aggravating it for the same number of upper-row subjects. At the
same time it introduces the additional stress-variable associated with cage
transfer (Mitchell and Gomber, 1976; Phoenix and Chambers, 1984; Crockett et
al., 1993; Schapiro et al., 1997). Even if techniques can be developed to assure
uniform illumination, the bottom tier cage will remain a potential source of
distress whenever personnel enters the room (cf., Kaumanns and Schönmann,
1997).
![]() |
Figure 13. Even though it is the prevailing housing arrangement, the double-tier caging system is unacceptable both for ethical and scientific reasons. |
In order to provide ethically and scientifically acceptable caging conditions, nonhuman primates must be housed in single-row cages to assure that (a) all animals receive the same quantity and quality of light, (b) all cages are of sufficient height so that occupants are in a position to retreat above animal care personnel, and (c) all animals in the room can be adequately inspected.
Training nonhuman primates to cooperate during procedures is one of the most significant options of making life a little bit more bearable for laboratory primates. It not only challenges the animals' high degree of intelligence, offers them –– and the caregivers –– some relevant distraction and eliminates data-confounding distress responses, but it also increases personnel safety by no longer giving the animals reason to defend themselves by means of biting or scratching during compulsory immobilization. To be forcefully removed from the familiar cage and subdued during painful husbandry and research procedures must, indeed, be a terribly frightening experience for a monkey or an ape. Research data collected from such an animal are tainted by the subject's stress reactions (review: Reinhardt et al., 1995) and, therefore, have questionable scientific value (Figure 14).
![]() | |
|
With gentle firmness, patience and
positive reinforcement many primate species can be conditioned to work with ––
rather than against –– personnel during common procedures such as transfer to a
holding area (Goodwin, 1997; Bloomsmith et al., 1998), capture from the home
cage (Reinhardt, 1992b), capture from the group (Reinhardt, 1990b,c;
Kessel-Davenport and Gutierrez, 1994; Mendoza, 1999; White et al., 2000), blood
collection (McGinnis and Kraemer, 1979; Reinhardt, 1991; Laule et al., 1996;
Moore and Suedmeyer, 1997), blood pressure measurement (Smith and Ansevin, 1957;
Mitchell et al., 1980; Turkkan, 1990), systemic injection (Spragg, 1940; Levison
et al., 1964; Byrd, 1977; Priest, 1991; Reinhardt, 1992c; Figure 15a,b), urine
collection (Kelly and Bramblett, 1981; Ziegler et al., 1987; Bond, 1991;
Anzenberger and Gossweiler, 1993; Shideler et al., 1994), saliva collection
(Bettinger, 1998; Bettinger et al., 1998), topical drug application (Reinhardt
and Cowley, 1990; Segerson and Laule, 1995), oral drug application (Turrkan et
al., 1989), semen collection (Brown and Loskutoff, 1998), insemination (Desmond
et al., 1987), vaginal swabbing (Bunyak et al., 1992; Hernándes-López et al.,
1998) and veterinary examination (Brown, 1998). The initial time investment in
the training quickly pays off in: (a) a reduction of time required to obtain a
sample, administer a drug or capture an animal, (b) a reduction of risks
associated with defense aggression, (c) a reduction in the use of
pharmacological restraint agents, (d) more reliable research data (Elvidge et
al., 1976; Reinhardt, 1992c; Schnell and Gerber, 1997; National Research
Council, 1998) and a more satisfactory relationship between handling personnel
and research subject (Figure 16a,b,c,d).
![]() |
![]() | |
|
Providing primates in research
institutions with primate-adequate housing and humane handling conditions is no
sentimentalism. On the contrary, it
is essential to employ such refined methodology in order to adhere to the very
basic principles of good science. A primate who behaves like a primate and who
is free of distress, certainly has a greater potential of being a useful
research model than one who is a behavioral cripple as a result of
understimulation and one who experiences distress during research-related
procedures.
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
Figure 16a,b,c,d. It takes less than a cumulative total of one hour to train adult male rhesus macaques to cooperate – rather than resist – during blood collection in the familiar home cage (Reinhardt, 1991). |
Abee CR 1985 Medical care and management of the squirrel monkey. In Handbook of Squirrel Monkey Research Rosenblum LA, Coe CL (ed), 447-488. Plenum Press, New York, NY
Anderson JR, Chamove AS 1984 Allowing
captive primates to forage. In Standards in Laboratory Animal Management.
Proceedings of a Symposium 253-256. The Universities Federation For Animal
Welfare, Potters Bar, UK
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/ufaw-2~1.htm
Anzenberger G, Gossweiler H 1993. How to obtain individual urine samples from undisturbed marmoset families. American Journal of Primatology 31, 223-230
Arluke A,Sanders C. R. 1996 Regarding Animals. Temple University Press, Philadelphia, PA
Baker KC 1997. Straw and forage material ameliorate abnormal behaviors in adult chimpanzees. Zoo Biology 16, 225-236
Baskerville M 1999 Old World Monkeys. In The UFAW Handbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Seventh Edition (edited by Poole, T. and English, P.), 611-635. Blackwell Science, Oxford, UK
Beck RPA 1995. A study of environmental enrichment in groups of captive lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia & Leontopithecus chrysmelas). RATEL (Journal of the Association of British Wild Animal Keepers) 22(4), 112-126
Bettinger T 1998 Saliva Collection of Trained Adult Male Gorillas (Videotape with commentary). In Workshop –– Advances in Primate Training. Twenty-first Annual Meeting of the American Society of Primatologists. O'Neill-Wagner P, Stone A (eds). Cleveland Metroparks Zoo, Cleveland, OH
Bettinger T, Kuhar C, Sironen A, Laudenslager M 1998. Behavior and salivary cortisol in gorillas housed in an all male group. American Zoo and Aquarium Association (AZA) Annual Conference Proceedings, 242-246
Bloomsmith MA, Stone AM, Laule GE 1998. Positive reinforcement training to enhance the voluntary movement of group-housed chimpanzees within their enclosure. Zoo Biology 17, 333-341
Boccia ML 1989. Long-term effects of a
natural foraging task on aggression and stereotypies in socially housed pigtail
macaques. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 28(2), 18-19
Full Text:
http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn28-2.html#maria
Bond M 1991. How to collect urine from a gorilla. Gorilla Gazette 5(3), 12-13
Boinski S, Swing SP, Gross TS, Davis JK 1999. Environmental enrichment of brown capuchins (Cebus apella): Behavioral and plasma and fecal cortisol measures of effectiveness. American Journal of Primatology 48, 49-68
Brent L 1992a. The effects of cage size and pair housing on the behavior of captive chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 27, 20
Brent L 1992b. Woodchip bedding as
enrichment for captive chimpanzees in an outdoor enclosure. Animal
Welfare 1, 161-170
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/aw1-1.htm
Brent L, Belik M 1997. The response of
group-housed baboons to three enrichment toys. Laboratory Animals 31,
81-85
Full Text: http://www.lal.org.uk/pdf.htm
Brockway BP, Hassler CR, Hicks N 1993 Minimizing stress during physiological monitoring. In Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing Niemi SM, Willson JE (eds), 56-69. Scientists Center for Animal Welfare, Bethesda, MD
Brown CS 1998 A Training Program for Semen Collection in Gorillas (Videotape with commentary). Ohama: Omaha's Henry Doorly Zoo.
Brown CS, Loskutoff NM 1998. A training program for noninvasive semen collection in captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Zoo Biology 17, 143-151
Brown DL, Calcagno J, Gold KC, Thompson S 1995. Effects of environmental enrichment on nonsocial and abnormal behavior of captive lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). American Zoo and Aquarium Association (AZA) Regional Conference Proceedings , 29-35
Bryant CE, Rupniak NMJ, Iversen SD 1988.
Effects of different environmental enrichment devices on cage stereotypies and
autoaggression in captive cynomolgus monkeys. Journal of Medical
Primatology 17, 257-269
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/jmp17-2.htm
Bunyak SC, Harvey NC, Rhine RJ, Wilson MI 1982. Venipuncture and vaginal swabbing in an enclosure occupied by a mixed-sex group of stumptailed macaques (Macaca arctoides). American Journal of Primatology 2, 201-204
Burt DA, Plant M 1990. Observations on a
caging system for housing stump-tailed macaques. Animal Technology 41,
175-179
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/at-burt.htm
Bushong D, Schapiro SJ, Bloomsmith MA 1992. Self-aggression in nonhuman primates: A review of its development/possible causes, methods of therapeutic treatment, and its relevance to the zoo situation. American Zoo and Aquarium Association (AZA) Regional Conference Proceedings , 723-728
Byrd LD 1977 Introduction: Chimpanzees as biomedical models. In Progress in Ape Research Bourne GH (ed), 161-165. Academic Press, New York, NY
Byrne GD, Suomi SJ 1991. Effects of woodchips and buried food on behavior patterns and psychological well-being of captive rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 23, 141-151
Byrum R, St. Claire M 1998. Pairing female
Macaca nemestrina. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 37(4),
1
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn37-4.html#byrum
Chamove AS, Anderson JR 1979. Woodchip
litter in macaque group. Animal Technology 30, 69-74
Full Text:
http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/at-cham.htm
Chamove AS, Anderson JR, Morgan-Jones SC,
Jones SP 1982. Deep woodchip litter: Hygiene, feeding, and behavioral
enhancement in eight primate species. International Journal for the Study of
Animal Problems 3, 308-318
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/ijsap1.html
Coe CL, Franklin D, Smith ER, Levine S 1982. Hormonal responses accompanying fear and agitation in the squirrel monkey. Physiology and Behavior 29, 1051-1057
Coelho AM, Carey KD, Shade RE 1991. Assessing the effects of social environment on blood pressure and heart rates of baboon. American Journal of Primatology 23, 257-267
Combette C, Anderson JR 1991. Réponses à deux techniques d'enrichissement environmental chez deux espèces de primates en laboratoire (Cebus apella, Lemur macaco). Cahiers d'Ethologie 11, 1-16
Crockett CM, Bielitzki JT, Carey A, Velez A
1989. Kong toys as enrichment devices for singly-caged macaques. Laboratory
Primate Newsletter 28(2), 21-22
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn28-2.html#kong
Crockett CM, Bowers CL, Sackett GP, Bowden DM 1993. Urinary cortisol responses of longtailed macaques to five cage sizes, tethering, sedation, and room change. American Journal of Primatology 30, 55-74
Desmond T, Laule GM, McNary J 1987. Training to enhance socialization and reproduction in drills. American Zoo and Aquarium Association (AZA) Regional Conference Proceedings , 352-358
Eaton GG, Kelley ST, Axthelm MK, Iliff-Sizemore SA, Shiigi SM 1994. Psychological well-being in paired adult female rhesus (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology 33, 89-99
Eckert K, Niemeyer C, Anonymous , Rogers RW,
Seier J, Ingersoll B, Barklay L, Brinkman C, Oliver S, Buckmaster C, Knowles L,
Pyle S 2000. Wooden objects for enrichment: A discussion. Laboratory Primate
Newsletter 39(3), 1-4
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn39-3.html#wood
Elvidge H, Challis JRG, Robinson JS, Roper C, Thorburn GD 1976. Influence of handling and sedation on plasma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Endocrinology 70, 325-326
Evans HL, Taylor JD, Ernst J, Graefe JF 1989. Methods to evaluate the well-being of laboratory primates. Comparison of macaques and tamarins. Laboratory Animal Science 39, 318-323
Fragaszy DM, Adams-Curtis LE 1991 Environmental challenges in groups of capuchins. In Primate Responses to Environmental Change Box HO (ed), 247-264. Chapman and Hall, New York, NY
Fritz P, Fritz J 1979. Resocialization of chimpanzees. Journal of Medical Primatology 8, 202-221
Gilloux I, Gurnell J, Shepherdson D 1992. An
enrichment device for great apes. Animal Welfare 1, 279-289
Full
Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/aw1-279.htm
Gonzalez CA, Coe CL, Levine S 1982. Cortisol responses under different housing conditions in female squirrel monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology 7, 209-216
Goodall J 1979 Anti-boredom devices for primates. In Comfortable Quarters for Laboratory Animals, Seventh Edition , 16. Animal Welfare Institute, Washington, DC
Goodwin J 1997. The application, use, and effects of training and enrichment variables with Japanese snow macaques (Macaca fuscata) at the Central Park Wildlife Center. American Zoo and Aquarium Association (AZA) Regional Conference Proceedings, 510-515
Grief L, Fritz J, Maki S 1992. Alternative
forage types for captive chimpanzees. Laboratory Primate Newsletter
31(2), 11-13
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn31-2.html#grief
Gwinn LA 1996. A method for using a pole housing apparatus to establish compatible pairs among squirrel monkeys. Contemporary Topics in Laboratory Animal Science 35(4), 61
Hamilton P 1991. Enrichment toys and tools in recent trials. Humane Innovations and Alternatives in Animal Experimentation 5, 272-277
Hartner M, Hall J, Penderghest J, Clark L P 2001. Group-housing subadult male cynomolgus macaques in a pharmaceutical environment. Lab Animal 30(8), 53-57
Heger W, Merker H-J, Neubert D 1986. Low
light intensity decreases the fertility of Callithrix jacchus. Primate
Report 14, 260
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/pr14-2.htm
Hernándes-López L, Mayagoitia L, Esquivel-Lacroix C, Rojas-Maya S, Mondragón-Ceballos R 1998. The menstrual cycle of the spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi). American Journal of Primatology 44, 183-195
Home Office 1989 Animals (Scientific
Procedures) Act 1986. Code of Practice for the Housing and Care of Animals Used
in Scientific Procedures. Her Majesty's Stationery Office, London,
UK
Full Text: http://www.homeoffice.gov.uk/animact/hcasp.htm
Howell SM, Mittra E, Fritz J, Baron J 1997.
The provision of cage furnishings as environmental enrichment at the Primate
Foundation of Arizona. The Newsletter 9(2), 1-5
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/jo-5.htm
International Primatological Society 1993.
IPS International guidelines for the acquisition, care and breeding of nonhuman
primates, Codes of Practice 1-3. Primate Report 35, 3-29
Full Text:
http://www.primate.wisc.edu/pin/ips2.html
Jackson MJ 2001. Environmental enrichment and husbandry of the MPTP-treated common marmoset. Animal Technology 52, 21-28
Jerome CP, Szostak L 1987. Environmental enrichment for adult, female baboons (Papio anubis). Laboratory Animal Science 37, 508-509
Jorgensen MJ, Kinsey JH, Novak MA 1998. Risk factors for self-injurious behavior in captive rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology 45, 187
König A, Rothe H, Radespiel U, Darms K, Bodemeyer J 1987. Nagehölzer für Krallenaffen. Zeitschrift des Kölner Zoo 30(3), 107-108
Kaumanns W, Schönmann U 1997. Requirements for cebids. Primate Report 49, 71-91
Kelley TM, Bramblett CA 1981. Urine collection from vervet monkeys by instrumental conditioning. American Journal of Primatology 1, 95-97
Kelly K 1993. Environmental enrichment for
captive wildlife through the simulation of gum feeding. Animal Welfare
Information Center Newsletter 4(3), 1-2 & 5-10
Full Text: http://www.nal.usda.gov/awic/newsletters/v4n3/4n3.htm
Kerridge FJ 1997. Behavioural enrichment of ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata) based upon a wild-captive comparison of their behaviour. Primate Eye 63, 36-37
Kessel AL, Brent L 1998. Cage toys reduce abnormal.behavior in individually housed pigtail macaques. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1, 227-234
Kessel-Davenport AL, Gutierrez T 1994.
Training captive chimpanzees for movement in a transport box. The
Newsletter 6(2), 1-2
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/jo-6.htm
Kopecky J, Reinhardt V 1991. Comparing the
effectiveness of PVC swings versus PVC perches as environmental enrichment
objects for caged female rhesus macaques. Laboratory Primate Newsletter
30(2), 5-6
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn30-2.html#vik
Laule GE, Thurston RH, Alford PL, Bloomsmith MA 1996. Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a diabetic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Zoo Biology 15, 587-591
Levison PK, Fester CB, Nieman WH, Findley JD 1964. A method for training unrestrained primates to receive drug injection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7, 253-254
Lindburg DG, Coe J 1995 Ark design update: Primate needs and requirements. In Conservation of Endangered Species in Captivity Gibbons EF, Durrant BS, Demarest AJ (eds), 553-570. SUNY Press, Albany, NY
Line SW, Markowitz H, Morgan KN, Strong S 1989 Evaluation of attempts to enrich the environment of single-caged non-human primates. In Animal Care and Use in Behavioral Research: Regulation, Issues, and Applications Driscoll JW (ed), 103-117. Animal Welfare Information Center National Agricultural Library, Beltsville, MD
Line SW, Morgan KN, Markowitz H, Roberts J,
Riddell M 1990. Behavioral responses of female long-tailed macaques (Macaca
fascicularis) to pair formation. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 29(4),
1-5
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn29-4.html#line
Lynch R 1998. Successful pair-housing of
male macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Laboratory Primate Newsletter
37(1), 4-5
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn37-1.html#pair
Macy JD, Beattie TA, Morgenstern SE, Arnstern AFT 1999. The use of guanfacine to control self-injurious behavior in nonhuman primates. Abstracts of the AALAS [American Association for Laboratory Animal Science] Meeting, 9
Mahoney CJ 1992. Some thoughts on psychological enrichment. Lab Animal 21(5), 27,29,32-37
Majolo B, Buchanan-Smith HM, Morris K 2001.
Factors affecting the successful pairing of unfamiliar common marmoset
(Callithrix jacchus) females. Primate Eye 73, 12-13
Full
Text: http://www.psgb.org/Meetings/Winter2000.html
Maki S, Alford PL, Bloomsmith MA, Franklin J 1989. Food puzzle device simulating termite fishing for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 19 (Supplement 1), 71-78
Markowitz H 1979 Environmental enrichment and behavioral engineering for captive primates. In Captivity and Behavior Erwin J, Maple T, Mitchell G (eds), 217-238. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, NY
Mason WA 1960. Socially mediated reduction in emotional responses of young rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 60, 100-110
McGinnis PR, Kraemer HC 1979 The Stanford outdoor primate facility. In Comfortable Quarters for Laboratory Animals, Seventh Edition, 20-27. Animal Welfare Institute, Washington, DC
McGrew WC, Brennan JARJ 1986. An artificial 'Gum-tree' for marmosets (Callithrix j. jacchus). Zoo Biology 5, 45-50
Mendoza SP 1999 Squirrel Monkeys. In The UFAW Handbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Seventh Edition UFAW [Universities Federation for Animal Welfare] (edited by Poole, T. and English, P.), 591-600. Blackwell Science, Oxford, UK
Mitchell DS, Wigodsky HS, Peel HH, McCaffrey TA 1980. Operant conditioning permits voluntary, noninvasive measurement of blood pressure in conscious, unrestrained baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation 12, 492-498
Mitchell G, Gomber J 1976. Moving laboratory rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) to unfamiliar home cages. Primates 17, 543-547
Molzen EM, French JA 1989 The problem of foraging in captive callitrichid primates: Behavioral time budgets and foraging skills. In Housing, Care and Psychological Wellbeing of Captive and Laboratory Primates Segal EF (ed), 89-101. Noyes Publications, Park Ridge, NJ
Moore BA, Suedmeyer K 1997. Blood sampling in 0.2 Bornean orangutans at the Kansas City Zoological Gardens. Animal Keepers' Forum 24, 537-540
Murchison MA 1994. Primary forage feeder for
singly-caged macaques. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 33(1),
7-8
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn33-1.html#mark
Murphy DE 1976. Enrichment and occupational
devices for orang utans and chimpanzees. International Zoo News
137(23.5), 24-26
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/izn-mur.htm
Nash VJ 1982. Tool use by captive chimpanzees at an artificial termite mound. Zoo Biology 1, 211-221
National Research Council 1998 The
Psychological Well-Being of Nonhuman Primates. National Academy Press,
Washington, DC
Full Text: http://pompeii.nap.edu/books/0309052335/html/index.html
Neveu H, Deputte BL 1996. Influence of availability of perches on the behavioral well-being of captive, group-living mangabeys. American Journal of Primatology 38, 175-185
Novak MA, Kinsey JH, Jorgensen MJ, Hazen TJ 1998. Effects of puzzle feeders on pathological behavior in individually housed rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 46, 213-227
Paquette D, Prescott J 1988. Use of novel objects to enhance environments of captive chimpanzees. Zoo Biology 7, 15-23
Perret K, Büchner S, Adler HJ 1998. Beschäftigungsprogramme für Schimpansen (Pan troglodytes) im Zoo. (Environmental enrichment program for chimpanzees in zoos) [German text with English summary]. Der Zoologische Garten 68, 95-111
Phoenix CH, Chambers KC 1984. Sexual behavior and serum hormone levels in aging rhesus males: Effects of environmental change. Hormones and Behavior 18, 206-215
Poenisch T 1992. Bedding for enrichment.
The Newsletter 4(1), 1
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/jo-14.htm
Preilowski B, Reger M, Engele H 1988. Combining scientific experimentation with conventional housing: A pilot study with rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 14, 223-234
Priest GM 1991. Training a diabetic drill
(Mandrillus leucophaeus) to accept insulin injections and venipuncture.
Laboratory Primate Newsletter 30(1), 1-4
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn30-1.html#loon
Pruetz JD, Bloomsmith MA 1992. Comparing two
manipulable objects as enrichment for captive chimpanzees. Animal Welfare
1, 127-137
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/aw1-12.htm
Ranheim S, Reinhardt V 1989. Compatible
rhesus monkeys provide long-term stimulation for each other. Laboratory
Primate Newsletter 28(3), 1-2
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn28-3.html#vik
Reese EP 1991 The role of husbandry in promoting the welfare of laboratory animals. In Animals in Biomedical Research Hendriksen CFM, Koeter HBWM (eds), 155-192. Elsevier, Amsterdam, Netherlands
Reinhardt V 1989a. Behavioral responses of
unrelated adult male rhesus monkeys familiarized and paired for the purpose of
environmental enrichment. American Journal of Primatology 17,
243-248
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn27-4.html#vik
Reinhardt V 1989b. Evaluation of the
long-term effectiveness of two environmental enrichment objects for singly caged
rhesus macaques. Lab Animal 18(6), 31-33
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/la-eval.htm
Reinhardt V, Houser WD, Eisele S 1989. Pairing previously singly caged rhesus monkeys does not interfere with common research protocols. Laboratory Animal Science 39, 73-74
Reinhardt V 1990a. Time budget of caged rhesus monkeys exposed to a companion, a PVC perch and a piece of wood for an extended time. American Journal of Primatology 20, 51-56
Reinhardt V 1990b. Avoiding undue stress:
Catching individual animals in groups of rhesus monkeys. Lab Animal
19(6), 52-53
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/la-avoid.htm
Reinhardt V 1990c Catching Individual
Rhesus Monkeys Living in Captive Groups (Videotape with commentary).
Wisconsin Regional Primate Research Center, Madison, WI
(Available on loan
from Animal Care Audio-Visual Materials, WRPRC, 1220 Capitol Court, Madison, WI
53715, USA)
Reinhardt V, Cowley D 1990. Training
stumptailed monkeys to cooperate during in-homecage treatment. Laboratory
Primate Newsletter 29(4), 9-10
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn29-4.html#vik
Reinhardt V 1991. Training adult male rhesus
monkeys to actively cooperate during in-homecage venipuncture. Animal
Technology 42, 11-17
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/at11.htm
Reinhardt V, Pape R 1991. An alternative
method for primate perch installation. Lab Animal 20(8), 47-48
Full
Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/la-an.htm
Reinhardt V, Reinhardt A 1991. Impact of a
privacy panel on the behavior of caged female rhesus monkeys living in pairs.
Journal of Experimental Animal Science 34, 55-58
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/es34-5~1.htm
Reinhardt V, Cowley D, Eisele S 1991. Serum
cortisol concentrations of single-housed and isosexually pair-housed adult
rhesus macaques. Journal of Experimental Animal Science 34,
73-76
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/es34-7~1.htm
Reinhardt V 1992a. Environmental enrichment
branches that do not clog drains. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 31(2),
8
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn31-2.html#branch
Reinhardt V 1992b. Transport-cage training
of caged rhesus macaques. Animal Technology 43, 57-61
Full Text:
http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/at57.htm
Reinhardt V 1992c Improved handling of
experimental rhesus monkeys. In The Inevitable Bond. Examining
Scientist-Animal Interactions Davis H, Balfour AD (eds), 171-177. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge, UK
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/bond.htm
Reinhardt V 1993a. Using the mesh ceiling as
a food puzzle to encourage foraging behaviour in caged rhesus macaques
(Macaca mulatta). Animal Welfare 2, 165-172
Full Text:
http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/aw3mesh.htm
Reinhardt V 1993b. Enticing nonhuman
primates to forage for their standard biscuit ration. Zoo Biology 12,
307-312
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/zb12-30.htm
Reinhardt V 1994a. Social enrichment for
previously single-caged stumptail macaques. Animal Technology 5,
37-41
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/at37.htm
Reinhardt V 1994b. Pair-housing rather than
single-housing for laboratory rhesus macaques. Journal of Medical
Primatology 23, 426-431
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/jmp23.htm
Reinhardt V 1994c. Caged rhesus macaques
voluntarily work for ordinary food. Primates 35, 95-98
Full Text:
http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/primat~1.htm
Reinhardt V, Liss C, Stevens C 1995.
Restraint methods of laboratory nonhuman primates: A critical review. Animal
Welfare 4, 221-238
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/aw6metho.htm
Reinhardt V 1997. The Wisconsin Gnawing
Stick. Animal Welfare Information Center (AWIC) Newsletter 7(3-4),
11-12
Full Text: http://www.nal.usda.gov/awic/newsletters/v7n3/7n3reinh.htm
Reinhardt V 1999. Pair-housing overcomes
self-biting behavior in macaques. Laboratory Primate Newsletter 38(1),
4
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn38-1.html#pair
Reinhardt V, Reinhardt A 1999. Are legal
cage space requirements sound? Laboratory Primate Newsletter 38(2),
5-6
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn38-2.html#cage
Reinhardt V, Garza-Schmidt M 2000. Daily
feeding enrichment for laboratory macaques: Inexpensive options. Laboratory
Primate Newsletter 39(2), 8-10
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn39-2.html#vik
Riviello MC, Misiti A 1995. An alternative
to woodchip as a foraging substrate for tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus
apella). Primate Report 42, 24
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/pr42-2~1.htm
Schapiro SJ, Bushong D 1994. Effects of
enrichment on veterinary treatment of laboratory rhesus macaques (Macaca
mulatta). Animal Welfare 3, 25-36
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/aw3-25.htm
Schapiro SJ, Nehete PN, Perlman JE, Sastry KJ 1997. A change in housing condition leads to relatively long-term changes in cell-mediated immune responses in adult rhesus macaques. American Journal of Primatology 42, 146
Schapiro SJ, Nehete PN, Perlman JE, Sastry KJ 2000. A comparison of cell-mediated immune responses in rhesus macaques housed singly, in pairs, or in groups . Applied Animal Behaviour Science 68, 67-84
Schapiro SJ, Stavisky R, Hook M 2000. The
lower-row cage may be dark, but behaviour does not appear to be affected.
Laboratory Primate Newsletter 39(1), 4-6
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn39-1.html#dark
Schnell CR, Gerber P 1997. Training and
remote monitoring of cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates. Primate
Report 49, 61-70
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/pr49-6~1.htm
Segerson L, Laule GE 1995. Initiating a training program with gorillas at the North Carolina Zoological Park. American Zoo and Aquarium Association (AZA) Annual Conference Proceedings, 488-489
Shideler SE, Savage A, Ortuño AM, Moorman EA, Lasley BL 1994. Monitoring female reproductive function by measurement of fecal estrogen and progesterone metabolites in the white-faced saki (Pithecia pithecia). American Journal of Primatology 32, 95-108
Shively CA 2001 Psychological Well-Being
of Laboratory Primates at Oregon Regional Primate Research Center.
Willamette Week, Portland, OR, March 21, 2001
Full Text: http://www.wweek.com/html2/shivreport.html
Smith A, Lindburg DG, Vehrencamp S 1989. Effect of food preparation on feeding behavior of lion-tailed macaques. Zoo Biology 8, 57-65
Smith CC, Ansevin A 1957. Blood pressure of the normal rhesus monkey. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine 96, 428-432
Spragg SDS 1940. Morphine addiction in chimpanzees. Comparative Psychology Monographs 15, 1-132
Sokol KA 1993. Commentary: Thinking like a monkey –– "primatomorphizing" an environmental enrichment program. Lab Animal 22(5), 40-45
Taylor WJ, Brown DA, Lucas-Awad J,
Laudenslager ML 1997. Response to temporally distributed feeding schedules in a
group of bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). Laboratory Primate
Newsletter 36(3), 1-3
Full Text: http://www.brown.edu/Research/Primate/lpn36-3.html#taylor
Turkkan JS 1990. New methodology for
measuring blood pressure in awake baboons with use of behavioral training
techniques. Journal of Medical Primatology 19, 455-466
Full Text:
http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/jmp19-4.htm
Turkkan JS, Ator NA, Brady JV, Craven KA 1989 Beyond chronic catheterization in laboratory primates. In Housing, Care and Psychological Wellbeing of Captive and Laboratory Primates Segal EF (ed), 305-322. Noyes Publications, Park Ridge, NJ
United States Department of Agriculture 2000
Animal Welfare Report –– Fiscal Year 2000. U.S. Department of Agriculture
–– Animal Care, Riverdale, MD
Full Text: http://www.aphis.usda.gov/ac/awrep2000.pdf
United States Department of Agriculture
1991. Title 9, CFR (Code of Federal Register), Part 3. Animal Welfare;
Standards; Final Rule. Federal Register 56(No. 32), 6426-6505
Full
Text: http://www.nal.usda.gov/awic/legislat/awadog.htm
de Waal FBM 1992 A social life for chimpanzees in captivity. In Chimpanzee Conservation and Public Health: Environments for the Future Erwin J, Landon JC (eds), 83-87. Diagnon/Bioqual, Rockville, MD
van Wagenen G 1950 The monkeys. In The Care and Breeding of Laboratory Animals Farris EJ (ed), 1-42. John Wiley, New York, NY
Wakenshaw V 1999. The management and
husbandry of Geoffroy's marmoset. International Zoo News 46(1),
3-15
Full Text: http://www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/izn-wak.htm
Watson DSB 1991. A built-in perch for primate squeeze cages. Laboratory Animal Science 41, 378-379
Weick BG, Perkins SE, Burnett DE, Rice TR, Staley EC 1991. Environmental enrichment objects and singly housed rhesus monkeys: Individual preferences and the restoration of novelty. Contemporary Topics in Laboratory Animal Science 30(5), 18
White G, Hill W, Speigel G, Valentine B, Weigant J, Wallis J 2000. Conversion of canine runs to group social housing for juvenile baboons. AALAS [American Association for Laboratory Animal Science] 51st National Meeting Official Program, 126
Williams LE, Abee CR, Barnes SR, Ricker RB 1988. Cage design and configuration for an arboreal species of primate. Laboratory Animal Science 38, 289-291
Wolfle TL 1987. Control of stress using non-drug approaches. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 191, 1219-1221
Ziegler TE, Bridson WE, Snowdon CT, Eman S 1987. Urinary gonadotropin and estrogen excretion during the postpartum estrus, conception, and pregnancy in the cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). American Journal of Primatology 12, 127-140
Viktor Reinhardt is Laboratory Animal Advisor to the Animal Welfare Institute in Washington, DC. He is a clinical veterinarian and ethologist and did extensive research in reproductive physiology, animal husbandry and ethology in cattle, muskox and bison.